Daniel Kahneman quotes:

+1
Share
Pin
Like
Send
Share
  • Happiness is determined by factors like your health, your family relationships and friendships, and above all by feeling that you are in control of how you spend your time.

  • If people are failing, they look inept. If people are succeeding, they look strong and good and competent. That's the 'halo effect.' Your first impression of a thing sets up your subsequent beliefs. If the company looks inept to you, you may assume everything else they do is inept.

  • People's mood is really determined primarily by their genetic make-up and personality, and in the second place by their immediate context, and only in the third and fourth place by worries and concerns and other things like that.

  • It is the consistency of the information that matters for a good story, not its completeness. Indeed, you will often find that knowing little makes it easier to fit everything you know into a coherent pattern.

  • It's nonsense to say money doesn't buy happiness, but people exaggerate the extent to which more money can buy more happiness.

  • I used to hold a unitary view, in which I proposed that only experienced happiness matters, and that life satisfaction is a fallible estimate of true happiness.

  • It's very easy for trusted companies to mislead naive customers, and life insurance companies are trusted.

  • By their very nature, heuristic shortcuts will produce biases, and that is true for both humans and artificial intelligence, but the heuristics of AI are not necessarily the human ones.

  • We think of our future as anticipated memories.

  • We have no reason to expect the quality of intuition to improve with the importance of the problem. Perhaps the contrary: high-stake problems are likely to involve powerful emotions and strong impulses to action.

  • There's a very good reason for why economics developed the way it did, and that is that in many situations, the assumption that people will exploit the opportunities available to them is very plausible, and it simplifies the analysis of how markets will behave.

  • Doubting what you see is a very odd experience. And doubting what you remember is a little less odd than doubting what you see. But it's also a pretty odd experience, because some memories come with a very compelling sense of truth about them, and that happens to be the case even for memories that are not true.

  • The average investor's return is significantly lower than market indices due primarily to market timing.

  • There's a tendency to look at investments in isolation. Investors focus on the risk of individual securities.

  • An investment said to have an 80% chance of success sounds far more attractive than one with a 20% chance of failure. The mind can't easily recognize that they are the same.

  • I would not advise people to buy a car or house without making a list. You will probably improve your intuitions by making a list and then sleeping on it.

  • I think one of the major results of the psychology of decision making is that people's attitudes and feelings about losses and gains are really not symmetric. So we really feel more pain when we lose $10,000 than we feel pleasure when we get $10,000.

  • Policy makers, like most people, normally feel that they already know all the psychology and all the sociology they are likely to need for their decisions. I don't think they are right, but that's the way it is.

  • You know, the standard state for people is 'mildly pleasant.' Negative emotions are quite rare, and extremely positive emotions are rare. But people are mildly pleased most of the time, they're mildly tired a lot of the time, and they wish they were somewhere else a substantial part of the time - but mostly they're mildly pleased.

  • We're beautiful devices. The devices work well; we're all experts in what we do. But when the mechanism fails, those failures can tell you a lot about how the mind works.

  • Human beings cannot comprehend very large or very small numbers. It would be useful for us to acknowledge that fact.

  • People talk of the new economy and of reinventing themselves in the workplace, and in that sense most of us are less secure.

  • The brains of humans contain a mechanism that is designed to give priority to bad news.

  • If owning stocks is a long-term project for you, following their changes constantly is a very, very bad idea. It's the worst possible thing you can do, because people are so sensitive to short-term losses. If you count your money every day, you'll be miserable.

  • We think, each of us, that we're much more rational than we are. And we think that we make our decisions because we have good reasons to make them. Even when it's the other way around. We believe in the reasons, because we've already made the decision.

  • People are really happier with friends than they are with their families or their spouse or their child.

  • One emphasis of my research has been on the question of how people spend their time. Time is the ultimate finite resource, or course, so the question of how people spend it would seem to be important.

  • Most of the time, we think fast. And most of the time we're really expert at what we're doing, and most of the time, what we do is right.

  • If you think in terms of major losses, because losses loom much larger than gains - that's a very well-established finding - you tend to be very risk-averse. When you think in terms of wealth, you tend to be much less risk-averse.

  • When you look at the books about well-being, you see one word - it's happiness. People do not distinguish.

  • We know that the French are very different from the Americans in their satisfaction with life. They're much less satisfied. Americans are pretty high up there, while the French are quite low - the world champions in life satisfaction are actually the Danes.

  • Yes, there is a burden of financial insecurity. I don't think you find it in mood. Income is correlated with life satisfaction, so maybe you do find it in life satisfaction. You don't find it in mood, and I think it is very important.

  • Suppose you like someone very much. Then, by a familiar halo effect, you will also be prone to believe many good things about that person - you will be biased in their favor. Most of us like ourselves very much, and that suffices to explain self-assessments that are biased in a particular direction.

  • Courage is willingness to take the risk once you know the odds. Optimistic overconfidence means you are taking the risk because you don't know the odds. It's a big difference.

  • In essence, the optimistic style involves taking credit for successes but little blame for failures.

  • People just hate the idea of losing. Any loss, even a small one, is just so terrible to contemplate that they compensate by buying insurance, including totally absurd policies like air travel.

  • My impression is that the elimination of memories greatly reduces the value of the experience.

  • There is research on the effects of 9/11, and you know, compared to the enormity of it, it didn't have a huge effect on people's mood. They were going about their business, mostly.

  • The experiencing self lives its life continuously. It has moments of experience, one after the other.

  • Through some combination of culture and biology, our minds are intuitively receptive to religion.

  • If there is time to reflect, slowing down is likely to be a good idea.

  • We're blind to our blindness. We have very little idea of how little we know. We're not designed to know how little we know.

  • My interest in well-being evolved from my interest in decision making - from raising the question of whether people know what they will want in the future and whether the things that people want for themselves will make them happy.

  • It's clear that policymakers and economists are going to be interested in the measurement of well-being primarily as it correlates with health; they also want to know whether researchers can validate subjective responses with physiological indices.

  • Political columnists and sports pundits are rewarded for being overconfident.

  • Many ideas happen to us. We have intuition, we have feeling, we have emotion, all of that happens, we don't decide to do it. We don't control it.

  • I'm not a great believer in self-help.

  • Poverty is clearly one source of emotional suffering, but there are others, like loneliness. A policy to reduce the loneliness of the elderly would certainly reduce suffering.

  • Optimistic people play a disproportionate role in shaping our lives. Their decisions make a difference; they are inventors, entrepreneurs, political and military leaders - not average people. They got to where they are by seeking challenges and taking risks.

  • I enjoy being active, but I look forward to the day when I can retire to the Internet.

  • People should be conscious of the large contribution made by anything that gets people together easily in the reduction of loneliness and emotional well-being.

  • Alternative descriptions of the same reality evoke different emotions and different associations.

  • All of us roughly know what memory is. I mean, memory is sort of the storage of the past. It's the storage of our personal experiences. It's a very big deal.

  • Divorced women, compared to married women, are less satisfied with their lives, which is not surprising. But they're actually more cheerful, when you look at the average mood they're in in the course of the day.

  • We don't only tell stories when we set out to tell stories, our memory tells us stories. That is, what we get to keep from our experiences is a story.

  • If individuals are rational, there is no need to protect them against their own choices.

  • People like leaders who look like they are dominant, optimistic, friendly to their friends, and quick on the trigger when it comes to enemies. They like boldness and despise the appearance of timidity and protracted doubt.

  • Experienced happiness refers to your feelings, to how happy you are as you live your life. In contrast, the satisfaction of the remembering self refers to your feelings when you think about your life.

  • Friends are sometimes a big help when they share your feelings. In the context of decisions, the friends who will serve you best are those who understand your feelings but are not overly impressed by them.

  • Nothing in life is quite as important as you think it is while you're thinking about it.

  • Nothing in life is as important as you think it is while you are thinking about it.

  • Slow thinking has the feeling of something you do. It's deliberate.

  • For many people, commuting is the worst part of the day, and policies that can make commuting shorter and more convenient would be a straightforward way to reduce minor but widespread suffering.

  • Adaptation seems to be, to a substantial extent, a process of reallocating your attention.

  • The idea that you can ask one question and it makes the point - well, that wasn't how psychology was done at the time.

  • It's a wonderful thing to be optimistic. It keeps you healthy and it keeps you resilient.

  • When everybody in a group is susceptible to similar biases, groups are inferior to individuals, because groups tend to be more extreme than individuals.

  • The psychologist, Paul Rozin, an expert on disgust, observed that a single cockroach will completely wreck the appeal of a bowl of cherries, but a cherry will do nothing at all for a bowl of cockroaches.

  • However, optimism is highly valued, socially and in the market; people and firms reward the providers of dangerously misleading information more than they reward truth tellers. One of the lessons of the financial crisis that led to the Great Recession is that there are periods in which competition, among experts and among organizations, creates powerful forces that favor a collective blindness to risk and uncertainty.

  • There are domains in which expertise is not possible. Stock picking is a good example. And in long-term political strategic forecasting, it's been shown that experts are just not better than a dice-throwing monkey.

  • When people think of the outcomes of their decisions, they think much more short term than that. They think in terms of gains and losses.

  • This is the essence of intuitive heuristics: when faced with a difficult question, we often answer an easier one instead, usually without noticing the substitution.

  • A person who has not made peace with his losses is likely to accept gambles that would be unacceptable to him otherwise.

  • We associate leadership with decisiveness. That perception of leadership pushes people to make decisions fairly quickly, lest they be seen as dithering and indecisive.

  • The 'Instagram Generation' now experiences the present as an anticipated memory

  • The illusion of skill is not only an individual aberration; it is deeply ingrained in the culture of the [investment management] industry.

  • Mind is a machine for jumping to conclusions

  • A reliable way to make people believe in falsehoods is frequent repetition, because familiarity is not easily distinguished from truth. Authoritarian institutions and marketers have always known this fact.

  • People exaggerate their confidence in their plans - something we call the planning fallacy... The existence of the plan tends to induce overconfidence.

  • The illusion that we understand the past fosters overconfidence in our ability to predict the future.

  • Except for some effects that I attribute mostly to age, my intuitive thinking is just as prone to overconfidence, extreme predictions, and the planning fallacy as it was before I made a study of these issues.

  • We're generally overconfident in our opinions and our impressions and judgments.

  • You should expect little or nothing from Wall Street stock pickers who hope to be more accurate than the market in predicting the future of prices. And you should not expect much from pundits making long-term forecasts.

  • When people believe a conclusion is true, they are also very likely to believe arguments that appear to support it, even when these arguments are unsound.

  • Doubts are suppressed by groups... But remember that the internal incentives that shape how the group perceives risks and rewards may be very different from the reality of the risks and rewards in the external marketplace. Those incentives can distort risk perception.

  • The idea that the future is unpredictable is undermined every day by the ease with which the past is explained.

  • However, attention can be moved away from an unwanted focus, primarily by focusing intently on another target.

  • The 'Instagram Generation' now experiences the present as an anticipated memory.

  • ...the characters are useful because of some quirks of our minds, yours and mine. A sentence is understood more easily if it describes what an agent (system 2) does than if it describes what something is, what properties it has.

  • Nothing in life is as important as you think it is when you are thinking about it.

  • The test of learning psychology is whether your understanding of situations you encounter has changed, not whether you have learned a new fact.

  • Jumping to conclusions is efficient if the conclusions are likely to be correct and the costs of an occasional mistake acceptable. Jumping to conclusions is risky when the situation is unfamiliar, the stakes are high and there is no time to collect more information.

  • The world makes much less sense than you think. The coherence comes mostly from the way your mind works.

  • Money does not buy you happiness, but lack of money certainly buys you misery.

  • People who are cognitively busy are also more likely to make selfish choices, use sexist language, and make superficial judgments in social situations.

  • If you're going to be unreligious, it's likely going to be due to reflecting on it and finding some things that are hard to believe.

  • Managers think of themselves as captains of a ship on a stormy sea. Risk for them is danger, but they are fighting it, very controlled.

  • Banks are run by executives, and executives protect themselves, and that does not always mean that banks are going to behave rationally.

  • There's a lot of randomness in the decisions that people make.

  • Psychologists really aim to be scientists, white-coat stuff, with elaborate statistics, running experiments.

  • Economists think about what people ought to do. Psychologists watch what they actually do.

  • People are very complex. And for a psychologist, you get fascinated by the complexity of human beings, and that is what I have lived with, you know, in my career all of my life, is the complexity of human beings.

  • Acquisition of skills requires a regular environment, an adequate opportunity to practice, and rapid and unequivocal feedback about the correctness of thoughts and actions.

  • The easiest way to increase happiness is to control your use of time.

  • Negotiations over a shrinking pie are especially difficult because they require an allocation of losses. People tend to be much more easygoing when they bargain over an expanding pie.

  • We have associations to things. We have, you know, we have associations to tables and to - and to dogs and to cats and to Harvard professors, and that's the way the mind works. It's an association machine.

  • We don't see very far in the future, we are very focused on one idea at a time, one problem at a time, and all these are incompatible with rationality as economic theory assumes it.

  • In a rising market, enough of your bad ideas will pay off so that you'll never learn that you should have fewer ideas.

  • When people talk of the economy being strong, they don't seem to feel that they, too, are better off.

  • If people do not know what is going to make them better off or give them pleasure, then the idea that you can trust people to do what will give them pleasure becomes questionable.

  • Nobody would say, 'I'm voting for this guy because he's got the stronger chin,' but that, in fact, is partly what happens.

  • After a crisis we tell ourselves we understand why it happened and maintain the illusion that the world is understandable. In fact, we should accept the world is incomprehensible much of the time.

  • Most successful pundits are selected for being opinionated, because it's interesting, and the penalties for incorrect predictions are negligible. You can make predictions, and a year later people won't remember them.

  • Intuitive diagnosis is reliable when people have a lot of relevant feedback. But people are very often willing to make intuitive diagnoses even when they're very likely to be wrong.

  • Optimism is normal, but some fortunate people are more optimistic than the rest of us. If you are genetically endowed with an optimistic bias, you hardly need to be told that you are a lucky person - you already feel fortunate.

  • Employers who violate rules of fairness are punished by reduced productivity, and merchants who follow unfair pricing policies can expect to lose sales.

  • People who know math understand what other mortals understand, but other mortals do not understand them. This asymmetry gives them a presumption of superior ability.

  • When people evaluate their life, they compare themselves to a standard of what a successful life is, and it turns out that standard tends to be universal: People in Togo and Denmark have the same idea of what a good life is, and a lot of that has to do with money and material prosperity.

  • I have always emphasized the willingness to discard.

  • The experiencing self lives in the moment; it is the one that answers the question, 'Does it hurt?' or 'What were you thinking about just now?' The remembering self is the one that answers questions about the overall evaluation of episodes or periods of one's life, such as a stay in the hospital or the years since one left college.

+1
Share
Pin
Like
Send
Share